Pocket worthyStories to fuel your mind

Babies Prefer the Sounds of Other Babies to the Cooing of Their Parents

Research indicates infants would rather listen to each other than to their parents’ babytalk.

The Conversation

Read when you’ve got time to spare.

file-20180508-34038-10vbrlt.jpg

Elena Stepanova / Shutterstock.com .

We know that babies prefer the high-pitched sounds produced by their caregivers in “babytalk” over regular speech, but a 2018 study provides an exciting new perspective. At five months of age, it seems that babies prefer to listen to the sounds of their peers to the cooing of their mother.

Researchers at the University of Quebec tested babies on their preference for different speakers by using a specialised speech synthesizer. They were able to simulate the effects of the human vocal tract – the vocal cords, tongue and mouth – to create vowels with differing pitch and resonance, representing vowels produced by vocal tracts of different sizes.

The apparatus let the researchers compare babies’ responses to vowels produced by infants their own age, as well as vowels typical of an adult female’s speech. They tested the babies’ responses to different vowel sounds by training them to look towards or away from a chequerboard image. Simply by turning their heads, the babies indicated which sounds they preferred.

The results were striking. Five-month-olds listened to the infant vowels for 40 percent longer than the adult vowels, showing a clear preference for vowels that closely matched the sounds they produce themselves.

These findings present a new view on how we think about babies’ early language learning. A lot of existing research focuses on the effect of parents’ speech on language development; for example, how words produced in a higher pitch grab babies’ attention more easily and go on to shape their early vocabulary. And there is no denying that babytalk is important in child development. Babies who hear more high-pitched babytalk from their caregivers have larger vocabularies at two years of age and higher IQs at age seven.

file-20180509-34018-1gyd6qg.jpg

You’re going to be a smart baby. Yes you are. Yes you are. Photo by Ekaterina Pokrovsky / Shutterstock.com

Babble Teaches Babies to Talk

But while babies prefer to listen to adult speech when it is produced at a higher pitch, a preference for infant vocalisations over and above this might have important implications for very early language learning. Authors of the study propose that it might motivate them to vocalise more in the first months of life, which could promote the transition to babble production just a few months later.

Babble is the emergence of repeated language-like syllables consisting of one or two “favourite” consonants, such as “bababa”. And we now know that it is an important indication of later language ability. Earlier onset of stable babbling leads to earlier word production and a larger vocabulary in the first two years of life. With this in mind, babies’ implicit preference for their own vocalisations could be an important factor in their path to full language use.

This is not the first study to suggest that infants’ own vocalisations may play an important role in language learning. Perception of consonants produced in their own babble may help infants filter the speech stream into something more manageable.

Studies have shown that infants prefer to listen to words that match the sounds produced in their babble. For example, an infant who produces many “bababa” sounds will prefer made-up words containing “b”, such as “bapeb”, “pabep” and “pobep”. This is similar to the “cocktail-party effect”, where even in a noisy room we can pick out words that are more relevant to us, such as our name or the town where we live.

In the same way, infants’ attention is drawn to words that match the sounds they produce most often, helping them pick out words from the speech stream that they’re more likely to be able to produce. It’s no coincidence that infants’ very first words have babble-like qualities: “mummy”, “daddy”, “baby” and onomatopoeic words such as “baa baa” and “woof woof”.

The importance of infants’ perception of their own vocalisations is supported by research into babies who are deaf. Studies of deaf babies have shown that they do babble but they start babbling much later than hearing infants.

Without feedback on their own babble production, deaf infants typically cease to babble after a few months. These studies have allowed us to piece together a more comprehensive picture of how human language emerges, taking into account the importance of infant vocalisation long before they produce their first word.

Babies learn from the adult world around them, but they also learn from their own early vocalisations. These new findings suggest that this begins much earlier than we previously thought. Perhaps language production does not start with words or even babble, but with vocalisations that begin long before the first speech-like sounds are produced.

Catherine Laing is a Lecturer in Child Language Acquisition at Cardiff University.

How was it? Save stories you love and never lose them.


Logo for The Conversation

This post originally appeared on The Conversation and was published May 10, 2018. This article is republished here with permission.

The Conversation brings you insights and analysis from academic experts, straight to your inbox.

Get the daily newsletter